Next Article in Journal
The Effects of a Novel Treatment for Hemianopic Dyslexia on Reading, Symptom Load, and Return to Work
Next Article in Special Issue
Dopamine Concentration Changes Associated with the Retrodialysis of Methylone and 3,4-Methylenedioxypyrovalerone (MDPV) into the Caudate Putamen
Previous Article in Journal
The Left Amygdala and Right Frontoparietal Cortex Support Emotional Adaptation Aftereffects
Previous Article in Special Issue
Effects of Serial Polydrug Use on the Rewarding and Aversive Effects of the Novel Synthetic Cathinone Eutylone
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Impacts of Self-Administered 3,4-Methylenedioxypyrovalerone (MDPV) Alone, and in Combination with Caffeine, on Recognition Memory and Striatal Monoamine Neurochemistry in Male Sprague Dawley Rats: Comparisons with Methamphetamine and Cocaine

by
Robert W. Seaman, Jr.
1,
Kariann Lamon
2,3,4,
Nicholas Whitton
2,3,4,
Brian Latimer
2,3,4,
Agnieszka Sulima
5,
Kenner C. Rice
5,
Kevin S. Murnane
2,3,4 and
Gregory T. Collins
1,6,*
1
Department of Pharmacology, The University of Texas Health Science Center at San Antonio, San Antonio, TX 78229, USA
2
Department of Pharmacology, Toxicology and Neuroscience, Louisiana State University Health Sciences Center at Shreveport, Shreveport, LA 71103, USA
3
Louisiana Addiction Research Center, Louisiana State University Health Sciences Center at Shreveport, Shreveport, LA 71103, USA
4
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Medicine, Louisiana State University Health Sciences Center at Shreveport, Shreveport, LA 71103, USA
5
Drug Design and Synthesis Section, Molecular Targets and Medications Discovery Branch, Intramural Research Program, National Institute on Drug Abuse and the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD 20892, USA
6
South Texas Veterans Health Care System, San Antonio, TX 78229, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2024, 14(3), 258; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14030258
Submission received: 1 February 2024 / Revised: 29 February 2024 / Accepted: 29 February 2024 / Published: 6 March 2024

Abstract

:
Recent data suggest that 3,4-methylenedioxypyrovalerone (MDPV) has neurotoxic effects; however, the cognitive and neurochemical consequences of MDPV self-administration remain largely unexplored. Furthermore, despite the fact that drug preparations that contain MDPV often also contain caffeine, little is known regarding the toxic effects produced by the co-use of these two stimulants. The current study investigated the degree to which self-administered MDPV or a mixture of MDPV+caffeine can produce deficits in recognition memory and alter neurochemistry relative to prototypical stimulants. Male Sprague Dawley rats were provided 90 min or 12 h access to MDPV, MDPV+caffeine, methamphetamine, cocaine, or saline for 6 weeks. Novel object recognition (NOR) memory was evaluated prior to any drug self-administration history and 3 weeks after the final self-administration session. Rats that had 12 h access to methamphetamine and those that had 90 min or 12 h access to MDPV+caffeine exhibited significant deficits in NOR, whereas no significant deficits were observed in rats that self-administered cocaine or MDPV. Striatal monoamine levels were not systematically affected. These data demonstrate synergism between MDPV and caffeine with regard to producing recognition memory deficits, highlighting the importance of recapitulating the manner in which drugs are used (e.g., in mixtures containing multiple stimulants, binge-like patterns of intake).

1. Introduction

Stimulants remain one of the most popular classes of drugs used worldwide. Upon examination of the novel psychoactive stimulants identified in the drug supply, a large proportion of them are synthetic cathinones. Synthetic cathinones emerged on the illicit drug market in the early 2000s in the form of “Bath Salts” preparations as popular “legal” alternatives to illicit psychostimulants, such as cocaine and methamphetamine, functioning in a similar manner as either inhibitors of monoamine reuptake or substrates for monoamine transporters [1,2,3,4,5,6]. Synthetic cathinones garnered much attention in the popular press due to a large number of adverse psychiatric (hallucinations, aggression, paranoia) and physiological (tachycardia, hyperthermia) effects associated with the use of various “Bath Salts” preparations [7,8,9,10]. One of the most commonly reported synthetic cathinones found in “Bath Salts” preparations is 3,4-methylenedioxypyrovalerone, or MDPV, a monoamine reuptake inhibitor that is ~750 times more potent at inhibiting reuptake at the dopamine transporter than the serotonin transporter [10,11,12,13]. Consistent with reports of MDPV being widely used, it has been demonstrated to share discriminative stimulus properties with other commonly used stimulants (e.g., cocaine, methamphetamine) [14,15,16,17,18,19], and it readily maintains self-administration, acting as a highly effective reinforcer in both rodents and non-human primates [11,20,21,22,23,24,25].
The extent to which the chronic use of synthetic cathinones could produce long-term toxicities, as is observed following the chronic use of other stimulants, is unclear. In vitro data suggest that MDPV can produce oxidative stress and mitochondrial dysfunction ultimately leading to apoptosis in SH-SY5Y cells [26,27,28,29,30]. Additionally, MDPV has been shown to produce cytotoxicity in bovine brain microvessel endothelial cells, suggesting that MDPV could produce deficits in blood–brain barrier integrity and/or function [31]. To date, the vast majority of studies investigating adverse effects of MDPV have employed either relatively short daily access sessions (e.g., 90 min) or non-contingent exposure. Though deficits in novel object recognition have been reported following a “binge” regimen of three non-contingent injections of 1 mg/kg MDPV per day, for ten days [32], case reports suggest that “Bath Salts” users tend to engage in repeated bouts of drug-taking behavior that can last days or even weeks [33,34,35,36,37]. Accordingly, incorporating longer access sessions (e.g., 12 h/day) to response-contingent MDPV might better model patterns of MDPV use reported by humans and aid in detailing possible long-term consequences of MDPV use. A study that employed a more translationally relevant model of MDPV exposure showed that intravenous self-administration of MDPV over five 96 h sessions was able to both disrupt recognition memory and increase markers of neurodegeneration in both the perirhinal and entorhinal cortices, relative to rats that only had access to saline infusions [38]. However, it remains unclear how those deficits produced by MDPV would compare to those in rats that had extended access to other stimulants (e.g., methamphetamine or cocaine), or if MDPV was administered in combination with other common “Bath Salts” constituents, such as caffeine. The latter is particularly important given the frequency with which MDPV is mixed with caffeine [10,12], as well as a number of studies that suggest caffeine is able to synergistically enhance the reinforcing, relapse-related, and discriminative stimulus effects of MDPV in rats [14,39,40].
The current studies begin to address these gaps in knowledge by integrating a 12 h access self-administration procedure with the novel object recognition assay and ex vivo neurochemical analyses in order to address the following hypotheses: (1) 12 h access to MDPV and MDPV+caffeine will result in escalations of intake akin to those observed in rats self-administering methamphetamine and cocaine; (2) 12 h access to methamphetamine, MDPV, and MDPV+caffeine will result in deficits in recognition memory; (3) 12 h access to methamphetamine, MDPV, and MDPV+caffeine will deplete striatal levels of dopamine; and (4) the addition of caffeine to MDPV will produce greater deficits in recognition memory and striatal dopamine neurochemistry than are observed in rats self-administering MDPV alone.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Self-Administration

2.1.1. Subjects

Eighty-four male Sprague Dawley rats (275–300 g upon arrival) were purchased from Envigo (Indianapolis, IN, USA) and maintained in a temperature- and humidity-controlled room. Rats were individually housed and maintained on a 14/10 h light/dark cycle. All experiments were conducted at approximately the same time each day. Rats were provided ad libitum access to Purina rat chow and water. All studies were carried out in accordance with the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committees of the University of Texas Health Science Center at San Antonio and the eighth edition of the Guide for Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (National Research Council (United States) Committee for the Update of the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals 2011).

2.1.2. Surgery

Rats were anesthetized with 2–3% isoflurane and prepared with chronic indwelling catheters in the left femoral vein, as previously described [23]. Catheters were tunneled under the skin and attached to a vascular access button placed in the mid-scapular region. Immediately following surgery, rats were administered EXCEDE® (Ceftiofur Crystalline Free Acid) (20 mg/kg; SC) to prevent infection. Rats were allowed 5–7 days to recover during which time catheters were flushed daily with 0.5 mL of heparinized saline (100 U/mL). Thereafter, catheters were flushed daily with 0.2 mL of saline prior to and 0.5 mL of heparinized saline after the completion of self-administration sessions.

2.1.3. Apparatus

Self-administration experiments were conducted in standard operant conditioning chambers located inside ventilated, sound-attenuating enclosures (Med Associates, Inc., St. Albans, VT, USA). Chambers were equipped with two response levers located 6.8 cm above the grid floor and 1.3 cm from the right or left wall. Visual stimuli were provided by two sets of green, yellow, and red LEDs, one set located above each of the two levers, and a white house light located at the top center of the opposite wall. Drug solutions were administered via variable-speed syringe pumps through Tygon tubing connected to a stainless-steel fluid swivel and spring tether, held in place by a counterbalanced arm. Experimental events were controlled, and data were collected using MED-PC IV software (Version MED-PC IV) and a PC-compatible interface (Med Associates, St. Albans, VT, USA).

2.1.4. Self-Administration

Rats were initially allowed to respond for the presentation of grain pellets (45 mg) under a fixed ratio (FR) 1 schedule of reinforcement during ten, daily, 90 min sessions. Illumination of green and red LEDs above the active lever (left or right; counterbalanced across rats) signaled grain pellet availability, and completion of the response requirement resulted in the delivery of a grain pellet, coinciding with the illumination of the house light as well as the yellow, green, and red LEDs above the active lever, and initiation of a 5 s timeout throughout which responding had no scheduled consequences. Upon the acquisition criteria (≥15 grain pellets delivered for two consecutive days with ≥80% responding occurring on the active lever versus the inactive lever) being met, a yellow LED was illuminated above the previously inactive lever signaling the availability of an infusion of either saline, cocaine (0.32 mg/kg/infusion), methamphetamine (0.1 mg/kg/infusion), MDPV (0.032 mg/kg/infusion), or a mixture of MDPV+caffeine (0.032 mg/kg/infusion and 0.11 mg/kg/infusion, respectively), under an FR 1 schedule of reinforcement during at least 10, daily, 90 min sessions. Completion of the response requirement resulted in a drug infusion (0.1 mL/kg over ~1 s), illumination of the house light as well as the yellow, green, and red LEDs above the active lever, and initiation of a 5 s timeout throughout which drug was made unavailable. Throughout the remainder of the study, responding on the lever signaled by green and red LEDs resulted in the delivery of a grain pellet, and responding on the alternate lever signaled by a yellow LED resulted in an infusion of drug (or saline). Response requirements for both grain pellets and drug (or saline) were subsequently increased to an FR 5 schedule where they remained for the duration of the study. Six days after rats were maintained under an FR 5 schedule of reinforcement, the session duration for half of the rats from the saline, cocaine, methamphetamine, MDPV, and MDPV+caffeine groups was increased from 90 min to 12 h; for the other rats, session durations remained at 90 min throughout. Self-administration sessions were conducted five days per week, for six weeks.

2.2. Drugs

Racemic MDPV was synthesized by Agnieszka Sulima and Kenner Rice (Bethesda, MD, USA) as previously described [41]. Cocaine was provided by the National Institute on Drug Abuse Drug Supply Program. D-methamphetamine and caffeine were purchased from Sigma-Aldrich (St. Louis, MO, USA). All drugs were dissolved in physiological saline.

2.3. Novel Object Recognition

Novel object recognition (NOR) was conducted in custom-built arenas with black walls and floors (43 cm × 56 cm × 38 cm; W × L × H). Webcams were situated above the arenas to record sessions for scoring at a later time. Rats underwent habituation, training, and test phases. During habituation, rats were allowed to freely explore an empty arena for 30 min (no objects were placed in the arena). Twenty-four hours later, a training session was conducted in which rats were placed into an arena with two identical objects placed on opposite sides of the arena, 20 cm apart, and allowed to explore the objects for 5 min before being returned to their home cages. The testing phase occurred 60 min later wherein one of the two identical objects (counterbalanced for left and right sides) was replaced by a different object (novel object), after which rats were allowed to explore both objects for 3 min. The objects used were a metallic can, an aluminum-foil-wrapped cardboard cone, a green striped aluminum-foil-wrapped cardboard cone, a plastic bottle, a miniature wooden birdhouse, and a miniature plastic sandcastle. Pilot studies suggested that rats had no preference for any single object. The objects acting as familiar or novel were pseudorandomized to aid in mitigating the influence of different objects resulting in additional variability. Exploration was defined as directing the nose toward the object at a distance of less than 2 cm [42]. The arena and the objects were cleaned between each trial with an alcohol-based cleaner. Recognition memory was quantified with an NOR score ([time spent with the novel object—time spent with the familiar object]/total exploration time). NOR was conducted prior to any drug self-administration and then three weeks following the final self-administration session. Throughout the 3-week drug-free period, rats remained in their home cages and were flushed daily.

2.4. Neurochemistry

To quantify monoamine neurochemistry in the striatum, rats were euthanized via rapid decapitation, and brains were removed. Brains were immediately frozen on dry ice and stored at −80 °C for subsequent analyses. Brains were thawed at 4 °C, placed in an ice-cold rat brain matrix, and sliced into coronal sections (1 mm thick). These slices were next placed flat on a cold plate over ice. Using a 1.5 mm diameter tissue biopsy punch, regions of interest were taken from individual slices, as described previously [43].
Frozen tissues were weighed, sonically disrupted in 100 µL of 0.3 N HClO4, and centrifuged for 10 min at 4 °C. A 100 µL aliquot of the supernatant was placed in a Waters 717plus autosampler maintained at 10 °C, and 50 µL was injected onto a Waters Spherisorb ODS 5 µm column, 4.6 × 250 mm (28 °C), 0.1 M sodium phosphate with 0.1 mM EDTA, 0.3 mM OSA, and 3.75% acetonitrile (pH adjusted to 3.05 with O-Phosphoric acid) running at a flow rate of 0.8 mL/min. Amperometric detection was accomplished with a BAS LC-4C detector equipped with a glassy carbon electrode (set to 780 mV), and the signal was analyzed using Waters Empower software (ver. 2). Absolute tissue concentrations (ng/mg) for the monoamine neurotransmitters dopamine, 3,4-dihydroxyphenylacetic acid (DOPAC), homovanillic acid (HVA), serotonin, 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (5-HIAA), norepinephrine, and epinephrine were determined by comparison with external standard curves and corrected for tissue weight, as described previously [43].

2.5. Data Analyses

Weekly intake data represent the mean (±S.E.M) level of drug (mg/kg) or food (pellets) earned across the 5 daily sessions. Intake data are plotted as a function of study week and were analyzed via two-way (access condition × experimental week) repeated-measure analysis of variance (ANOVA) and post hoc Holm–Sidak test. Daily intake data are presented as the mean ± S.E.M. Total experimental intake data are presented for individual subjects and were analyzed via Student’s t-tests (90 min versus 12 h access). Novel object recognition scores, total exploration time, and striatal dopamine neurochemistry data are shown for individual subjects and were analyzed via two-way (access condition x experimental week) ANOVA and post hoc Holm–Sidak test. Given the relatively small quantities of norepinephrine and serotonin in the striatum, and their respective metabolites, these neurochemistry data are described and shown but not analyzed statistically to mitigate the conflation of statistical significance with biological significance. GraphPad Prism 10 software (GraphPad Software Inc., La Jolla, CA, USA) was used to conduct statistical analyses and plot figures.

3. Results

3.1. Self-Administration

The average daily intakes for each reinforcer during 90 min or 12 h access sessions across each of the 6 experimental weeks are presented in Figure 1. Overall, there were main effects of session duration, with rats provided 12 h access to drug achieving greater average levels of drug intake per session relative to rats provided 90 min access, revealed by repeated-measure ANOVAs with regard to cocaine (F(1,15) = 22.98, p < 0.05; upper left), methamphetamine (F(1,16) = 17.62, p < 0.05; upper right), MDPV in rats that had access to MDPV alone (F(1,15) = 11.56, p < 0.05; middle left), MDPV in rats that had access to a mixture of MDPV+caffeine (F(3,26) = 18.1, p < 0.05; middle right), caffeine in rats that had access to a mixture of MDPV+caffeine (F(3,26) = 18.1, p < 0.05; bottom left), and grain pellets (F(1,15) = 11.56, p < 0.05; bottom right).
When evaluated based on average intake per session as a function of experimental week, cocaine self-administration remained stable over the course of the experiment, regardless of access condition, with no main effect of experimental week (F(5,75) = 0.47, n.s.). Although there was no main effect of experimental week (F(5,80) = 0.81, n.s.) average methamphetamine intake per session decreased over the course of the study in rats provided 12 h access, but slightly increased in rats provided 90 min access.
The opposite was true for MDPV, in that rats provided 12 h access increased their average session intake over the course of the study, whereas rats provided 90 min access slightly decreased their MDPV intake, with no main effect of experimental week observed (F(5,75) = 0.67, n.s.). Rats responding for MDPV+caffeine earned fewer overall infusions than rats self-administering MDPV alone, meaning less MDPV intake. Intake of both MDPV and caffeine in rats self-administering a mixture of both drugs resulted in an initial increase in drug intake followed by a decrease over the course of the experiment. There was a significant effect of experimental week for intake of MDPV (F(5,65) = 3.1, p < 0.05) as well as of caffeine (F(5,65) = 3.1, p < 0.05). Post hoc tests revealed significant increases in average MDPV and caffeine intake per session between weeks 1 and 2 (p < 0.05). Twelve-hour access to grain pellet delivery resulted in an overall decrease over the course of the experiment, although there was no significant effect of experimental week (F(5,75) = 1.6, n.s.).
Perhaps unsurprisingly, total drug intake (i.e., over the course of the entire study) was significantly greater in rats having 12 h access to cocaine (t = 5.7, p < 0.05), methamphetamine (t = 4.2, p < 0.05), MDPV (alone) (t = 3.6, p < 0.05), MDPV (MDPV+caffeine) (t = 4.1, p < 0.05), caffeine (MDPV+caffeine) (t = 4.1, p < 0.05), and grain pellets (t = 5.2, p < 0.05) relative to rats that were provided 90 min access.
To examine how drug-taking patterns change from day to day, and whether these patterns differ depending on the drug(s) being self-administered, daily intake data are shown in Figure 2. Cocaine self-administration minimally fluctuates across days over the course of the study, regardless of access condition. Rats self-administering methamphetamine, however, self-administered more of the drug earlier in the experimental week and then decreased drug intake over the course of the week.
Although this is more apparent in rats provided 12 h access to methamphetamine, a similar pattern can be observed, particularly in week 6, in rats provided only 90 min access to methamphetamine. Twelve-hour access to MDPV initially resulted in rats increasing daily intake for the first two weeks and then transitioning to a pattern of intake not dissimilar from that for methamphetamine (i.e., more drug intake earlier in the week than towards the end). Similarly, by week three, rats self-administering MDPV in 90 min sessions began self-administering more of the drug early in the week and then reduced their intake over the course of the week. Twelve-hour access to MDPV+caffeine resulted in far less MDPV intake, but patterns of responding were similar. Rats self-administered more of the drug early in the week, followed by a subsequent decrease, and then often an increase by the end of the week (i.e., a “V” shaped pattern of intake within an experimental week). Rats responding for grain pellets exhibited relatively stable levels of responding across days over the course of the study, regardless of access condition.

3.2. Novel Object Recognition

Recognition memory was probed at baseline (i.e., before any drug self-administration) and following a 3-week drug-free period. Two-way repeated-measure ANOVAs revealed significant main effects of experimental week in rats that had self-administered methamphetamine (F(1,16) = 7.2, p < 0.05) (Figure 3; upper right) or a mixture of MDPV+caffeine (F(1,25) = 10.4, p < 0.05) (Figure 3; middle right), with post hoc tests revealing a significant decrease in NOR score from baseline in rats that had 12 h access to methamphetamine and in rats that had either 90 min or 12 h access to MDPV+caffeine. In contrast, no main effects of experimental week on NOR score were observed in rats that had access to cocaine (F(1,17) = 0.30, n.s.) (Figure 3; upper left), grain pellets (F(1,14) = 0.14, n.s.) (Figure 3; bottom left), or MDPV (F(1,15) = 0.3, n.s.) (Figure 3; middle left).
Significant main effects of experimental week with regard to total exploration time were observed in rats that had self-administered MDPV (F(1,15) = 13.1, p < 0.05) or MDPV+caffeine (F(1,25) = 9.5, p < 0.05), with post hoc tests revealing significant increases in total exploration time (p < 0.05) in rats that had either 90 min or 12 h access to MDPV, or 12 h access to MDPV+caffeine.

3.3. Striatal Monoamine Neurochemistry

3.3.1. Dopamine

The long-term effects of 90 min or 12 h access to each reinforcer on striatal dopamine neurochemistry were assessed following a 3-week drug-free period (Figure 4). There was a significant main effect of a drug on levels of dopamine (F(4,77) = 2.6, p < 0.05), with post hoc tests revealing a significant decrease in dopamine in rats allowed 90 min access to cocaine, relative to those that had 90 min access to grain pellets (Figure 4; top). No other significant changes in dopamine levels were observed as a function of self-administration of any other drug. With regard to the dopamine metabolite, 3,4-dihydroxyphenylacetic acid (DOPAC), there was a significant main effect of access condition (F(1,77) = 7.4, p < 0.05) and an interaction between access condition and drug (F(4,77) = 2.9, p < 0.05); however, post hoc tests revealed no significant differences in DOPAC levels in the striatum of rats that had either 90 min or 12 h access to any drug versus those having access to grain pellets (Figure 4; upper middle). A two-way ANOVA also revealed significant main effects of both access condition (F(1,73) = 84, p < 0.05) and drug (F(4,73) = 15.1, p < 0.05), as well as an interaction (F(4,73) = 13.9, p < 0.05) on striatal levels of homovanillic acid (HVA) (Figure 4; lower middle). Post hoc analyses revealed significant decreases in HVA following 90 min access to cocaine, MDPV, or MDPV+caffeine, whereas 90 min access to methamphetamine resulted in a significant increase in HVA, relative to levels in rats responding for grain pellets. There were no significant differences in HVA levels following 12 h access to any drug, relative to grain pellets. Finally, with regard to dopamine turnover (i.e., levels of DOPAC/levels of dopamine), there were significant main effects of both access condition (F(1,77) = 17.8, p < 0.05) and drug (F(4,77) = 6.1, p < 0.05), as well as an interaction (F(4,77) = 2.9, p < 0.05) (Figure 4; bottom). Post hoc analyses revealed a significant increase in dopamine turnover in rats allowed 90 min access to methamphetamine, relative to those responding for grain pellets.

3.3.2. Norepinephrine

Apparent increases in striatal norepinephrine levels were observed in rats allowed 90 min or 12 h access to methamphetamine, relative to those that had 90 min access to grain pellets (Table 1). With regard to striatal levels of epinephrine, seemingly small reductions were observed in rats that had 90 min access to any of the drugs, as compared to rats that had 90 min access to grain pellets. In contrast, only 12 h access to methamphetamine and MDPV+caffeine resulted in apparent decreases in striatal epinephrine (Table 1). With regard to norepinephrine turnover (i.e., levels of epinephrine/levels of norepinephrine), there were apparent increases in norepinephrine turnover in rats allowed 90 min access to any of the drugs, relative to those that had access to grain pellets. In contrast, 12 h access to any of the drugs did not appear to produce large differences in norepinephrine turnover compared to what was observed in rats responding for grain pellets in 12 h sessions (Table 1).

3.3.3. Serotonin

When levels of striatal serotonin were assessed, there was an apparent reduction in serotonin observed for rats allowed 90 min or 12 h access to any of the drugs, relative to those that had access to grain pellets (Table 1). Unlike what was observed with serotonin, none of the drug access conditions appeared to affect the striatal levels of the serotonin metabolite, 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (5-HIAA) (Table 1). With regard to serotonin turnover (i.e., levels of 5-HIAA/levels of serotonin), slight increases were observed for rats allowed 12 h access to any of the drugs, relative to those responding for grain pellets. In contrast, when levels of striatal serotonin turnover were compared for rats from the 90 min access conditions, there were no apparent differences, regardless of reinforcer (Table 1).

4. Discussion

Although MDPV has been reported to be one of the most prominent synthetic cathinones in “Bath Salts” preparations [10,12,13], relatively little is known regarding the consequences of its use. The current studies utilized an uncapped, 12 h access self-administration paradigm to compare patterns of drug intake, changes in recognition memory, and alterations in striatal monoamine neurochemistry in rats self-administering MDPV, cocaine, or methamphetamine. In addition, because “Bath Salts” preparations often contain other psychoactive substances, such as caffeine, these studies also included a mixture of MDPV+caffeine in order to determine if the presence of caffeine altered the behavioral and neurochemical consequences of MDPV self-administration. There were five main findings: (1) patterns of self-administration were drug-dependent, with cocaine maintaining relatively stable levels of responding across an experimental week, whereas rats self-administering methamphetamine, MDPV, and MDPV+caffeine exhibited “binge-like” patterns of drug intake, self-administering larger amounts of drug earlier in the week versus later in the week; (2) the addition of caffeine to MDPV resulted in less MDPV intake than that observed for rats provided access to the same dose of MDPV in isolation; (3) 12 h access to methamphetamine resulted in deficits in recognition memory not observed in rats provided 12 h access to cocaine or MDPV; (4) 90 min and 12 h access to a mixture of MDPV and caffeine resulted in deficits in recognition memory, despite less overall MDPV intake; and (5) overall, striatal monoamine neurochemistry was not systematically altered as a function of the access condition, the drug being self-administered, or the levels of intake. Taken together, these data suggest that extended access to MDPV alone is insufficient to produce long-lasting deficits in recognition memory or changes in monoamine neurochemistry in the striatum, whereas the addition of caffeine to self-administered MDPV can result in cognitive deficits, even in rats allowed only 90 min access per day, highlighting the importance of evaluating drug mixtures rather than just single drugs, in isolation, for drugs that are commonly co-used.
In an attempt to recapitulate the “binge-and-crash” patterns often reported in human stimulant users, the current studies allowed rats uncapped access to cocaine, methamphetamine, MDPV, or MDPV+caffeine, during either 90 min or 12 h sessions conducted five days per week, for six weeks. Regarding patterns of intake across a given experimental week, cocaine intake tended to be relatively stable across the course of the experiment, with similar levels of intake occurring each session. This was in contrast to what was observed in rats self-administering methamphetamine, MDPV, or MDPV+caffeine in 12 h sessions, wherein intake was unevenly distributed throughout a given experimental week (e.g., greater levels of drug intake early in the week followed by days of far less drug intake), possibly recapitulating some aspects of stimulant use patterns in humans. Of note, rats allowed access to a mixture of MDPV+caffeine self-administered less MDPV than rats allowed access to MDPV alone. Although the drivers of this pattern of intake, and why it seemingly did not develop in rats self-administering cocaine, are outside of the scope of this study, it is likely the culmination of large levels of drug intake that disrupted a plethora of physiological (e.g., circadian, feeding, cardiovascular) functions. Perhaps during sessions of little to no drug intake, these physiological functions were restored sufficiently that drug intake would be greater the following day. This pattern of intake largely recapitulates the “binge-and-crash” patterns of stimulant use reported in human users. As mentioned previously, few studies have allowed rats greater than 6 h access to methamphetamine, and those that have typically either cap the number of infusions that can be earned in a single session or integrate periods of drug unavailability (e.g., 12 h access every other day, 1 h drug-free period every 3 h), perhaps reducing the likelihood of such patterns of intake developing.
One study that has evaluated extended access to MDPV self-administration, wherein rats were allowed to self-administer MDPV within five 96 h sessions each separated by a three-day drug-free period, found that MDPV intake was demonstrated to significantly increase over time relative to that observed for rats that had access to infusions of saline. However, it should be noted that rats had not previously acquired self-administration behavior prior to starting the first 96 h self-administration session that intake in subsequent sessions was compared to, likely contributing to the “escalation of intake” that was observed [38]. Those data were presented as intake per 96 h session, so the resolution with which one can compare those data with the current data with regard to patterns of intake across 12 h is limited. In the current studies, self-administration of a mixture of MDPV+caffeine (0.032 mg/kg/inf + 0.11 mg/kg/inf, respectively) resulted in fewer infusions being earned and therefore less MDPV intake than self-administration of MDPV (0.032 mg/kg/inf) alone. This is likely due to MDPV and caffeine, at these doses, having an additive effect with regard to reinforcing effects [40] (i.e., the mixture of MDPV+caffeine acted as a functionally larger reinforcer, maintaining fewer infusions under a fixed ratio schedule of reinforcement, akin to what would be observed if a larger MDPV dose was made available) or due to the amount of caffeine being self-administered having rate-decreasing effects. It should be noted that 12 h access to MDPV+caffeine proved to be lethal in two rats, whereas no other condition in the current studies resulted in lethality. In these two subjects, total caffeine intake averaged ~100 mg/kg in 12 h for multiple sessions before overdosing. Lethality resulting from that level is consistent with the LD50 reported for intravenous caffeine, 105 mg/kg [44]. Our laboratory and others have demonstrated that intravenous caffeine, when self-administered in isolation, does not maintain intake levels that are consistent with a caffeine overdose, suggesting that the addition of MDPV aided in increasing caffeine intake to the point of reaching potentially lethal levels [45,46,47,48]. In addition, MDPV and caffeine could interact synergistically with regard to toxic effects, decreasing the dose of either drug necessary to be lethal. Nevertheless, given that few studies have provided rats with extended access to caffeine alone, and the current studies only evaluated one mixture of MDPV+caffeine, future work should better characterize toxic interactions between MDPV and caffeine using extended-access self-administration as an exposure paradigm.
Because long-term use of stimulants has been linked to cognitive deficits across a number of domains, the current studies sought to directly compare the impact of self-administered methamphetamine, cocaine, MDPV, and MDPV+caffeine on cognition. Though these effects tend to be more prominent with methamphetamine than cocaine, preclinical data suggest the use of MDPV can result in cognitive deficits. Thus, to examine the lasting consequences of various stimulant self-administration histories on cognitive performance related to recognition memory, novel object recognition was evaluated three weeks following the final self-administration session. Consistent with previous reports, 90 min or 12 h access to cocaine did not produce deficits in recognition memory, whereas 12 h (but not 90 min) access to methamphetamine did [49,50,51,52,53] (but see [54]). This is consistent with reports that drugs acting as reuptake inhibitors produce fewer cognitive deficits relative to those acting as substrates for monoamine transporters. Similar to what was observed with cocaine, and consistent with its mechanism of action as a monoamine reuptake inhibitor, self-administered MDPV alone (in either 90 min or 12 h sessions) did not produce deficits in recognition memory relative to the pre-drug baseline. This is in contrast to a previous report in rats wherein blunted recognition memory was observed following multiple 96 h self-administration sessions wherein MDPV was available [38]. This discrepancy could be the result of differences in drug availability (96 h sessions versus 12 h sessions), using rats that had access to saline as a comparator rather than rats that responded for grain pellets (i.e., controlling for a history of operant behavior), or in the time allowed to explore the two identical objects during the training phase of the novel object recognition assay prior to the testing phase (3 min versus 5 min). Previous work suggests that the lack of an effect of 12 h access to either reuptake inhibitor on recognition memory could be due to the task not requiring enough of a cognitive burden to unveil deficits in memory. Indeed, it has been shown that 6 h access to cocaine produced deficits in recognition memory when the interval between the training and test phases was 24 h, but not when the interval between training and test phases was only 3 h [55,56]. Future work should use a battery of cognitive assays to attain better resolution in characterizing functional cognitive deficits following stimulant self-administration.
Interestingly, a history of either 90 min or 12 h self-administration of a mixture of MDPV+caffeine did result in deficits in recognition memory, relative to baseline, with the caveat that the average pre-drug baseline NOR score in these subjects was larger than that in other groups. Although identifying the underlying mechanisms contributing to synergism between MDPV and caffeine with regard to persistent effects on memory is outside the scope of these studies, the idea that synergism between MDPV and caffeine exists with regard to producing memory deficits is consistent with previous work demonstrating synergism between these two drugs with regard to reinforcing effects [40] and discriminative stimulus effects [14,39]. Although it cannot be entirely ruled out based on the current studies, it is unlikely that self-administered caffeine, alone, was able to produce deficits in recognition memory given the body of work demonstrating either no change or enhancements in cognition rather than deficits following caffeine administration [57,58,59,60,61,62,63,64,65,66,67,68,69]. One potential mechanism underlying the apparent synergism between MDPV and caffeine in producing deficits in recognition memory is via enhancements in dopamine accumulation through the ability of caffeine to disinhibit dopamine D2 signaling. Perhaps this resulted in some degree of dopamine toxicity often associated with drugs acting as substrates for monoamine transporters (e.g., methamphetamine), wherein accumulated dopamine can form superoxide radicals and hydrogen peroxide that can result in neurodegeneration in regions associated with recognition memory behavior (e.g., hippocampus) (for a review, see [70]). However, as previously mentioned, self-administration of caffeine, in isolation, would likely result in far less caffeine intake than what was observed in the current studies when caffeine was self-administered in conjunction with MDPV, making it challenging to parse apart the individual contribution of caffeine, but something that should be further investigated.
In an attempt to identify potential mechanisms underlying the behavioral changes, changes to striatal monoamine neurochemistry produced by each drug as a function of access condition were evaluated. With regard to the dopaminergic system, levels of striatal dopamine and DOPAC were largely unchanged independent of access condition or reinforcer available. There were small but significant reductions in HVA in rats that had 90 min access to cocaine, MDPV, or a mixture of MDPV+caffeine, whereas 90 min access to methamphetamine resulted in an increase in HVA, relative to rats with a history of 90 min access to grain pellets. Ninety-minute access to methamphetamine also resulted in a significant increase in dopamine turnover. It should be noted that there was a significant negative correlation between methamphetamine intake and dopamine levels; however, no other such correlation was observed with regard to other reinforcers, or other neurotransmitters, suggesting that it is not simply variability with regard to drug intake responsible for the lack of effects observed. Whereas dopamine neurochemistry was largely unchanged, all drugs appeared to reduce levels of striatal serotonin regardless of access condition, without large changes in 5-HIAA levels, although serotonin turnover appeared to increase in rats allowed 12 h access to MDPV+caffeine. Lastly, norepinephrine levels appeared to be greater in rats that had self-administered methamphetamine in both 90 min and 12 h access sessions, relative to those responding for grain pellets, whereas access to any other drug appeared to be without effect on norepinephrine levels. In contrast, 90 min access to all drugs and 12 h access to methamphetamine resulted in an apparent decrease in epinephrine levels, accompanied by reductions in norepinephrine turnover. Generally, the seeming lack of large effects observed on striatal monoamine neurochemistry following thirty days of drug-taking, particularly in rats provided 12 h access, was unexpected. Methodological differences make comparisons with non-contingent “binge” models difficult. For instance, differences in duration of access per session (e.g., 90 min, 6 h, 12 h), total experimental duration, and time between the final self-administration session and tissue collection likely all contribute to the range of effects observed across studies. Indeed, the current studies utilized a 3-week drug-free period prior to tissue collection, to capture long-lasting persistent effects on neurochemistry. Previous work suggests that the duration of this period might preclude observing large changes in striatal neurochemistry that have been observed following a shorter drug-free period [71,72,73,74]. In addition to striatal monoamine neurochemistry, monoamine neurochemistry, neurodegeneration, and neuroinflammation will be quantified in other brain regions (hippocampus, prefrontal cortex) that are associated with recognition memory, to allow for a deeper characterization of the persistent effects of self-administered synthetic cathinones and what could be driving the deficits in novel object recognition observed.

5. Conclusions

Despite the prevalence of MDPV and caffeine in “Bath Salts” preparations, relatively little is known regarding the persistent consequences of their use. The current studies found that rats allowed 90 min or 12 h access to MDPV+caffeine or 12 h access to methamphetamine exhibited deficits in recognition memory, whereas rats allowed to respond for cocaine or MDPV did not. These findings are in line with what has been previously observed in rats allowed to self-administer methamphetamine and suggest that MDPV and caffeine interact not only to enhance abuse-related endpoints (e.g., discrimination, reinforcement, and reinstatement) but also adverse effects ranging from cognitive impairment to lethality. Generally, there were few changes to striatal monoamine neurochemistry, suggesting that monoamine levels recovered over the 3-week drug-free period or that physiological systems were able to compensate for drug effects. Future work will use extended-access self-administration as an exposure paradigm to investigate the effects of chronic self-administered stimulants and stimulant mixtures on cardiovascular parameters and stimulant pharmacokinetics with the ultimate goal of developing novel treatments to prevent and/or rescue deficits produced by chronic stimulant use.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, R.W.S.J. and G.T.C.; methodology, R.W.S.J., K.S.M. and G.T.C.; formal analysis, R.W.S.J. and G.T.C.; investigation, R.W.S.J., K.L., N.W. and B.L.; reagents, A.S. and K.C.R.; writing—original draft preparation, R.W.S.J. and G.T.C.; writing—review and editing, R.W.S.J., K.S.M. and G.T.C. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by research grants from the US National Institutes of Health/National Institute on Drug Abuse, grant number R01DA039146 (GTC). The work of the Drug Design and Synthesis Section of the Molecular Targets and Medications Discovery Branch was supported by the NIH Intramural Research Programs of NIDA and NIAAA.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the UTHSCSA IACUC (protocol # 20200020AR) on 30 April 2020 with a current expiration date of 29 May 2024.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article; further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Melson Mesmin, Rachel DeSantis, Torie Campos, Juan Morales, and Karen Jimenez for their technical assistance in the completion of these studies.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Baumann, M.H.; Partilla, J.S.; Lehner, K.R. Psychoactive “Bath Salts”: Not so Soothing. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 2013, 698, 1–5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Baumann, M.H.; Partilla, J.S.; Lehner, K.R.; Thorndike, E.B.; Hoffman, A.F.; Holy, M.; Rothman, R.B.; Goldberg, S.R.; Lupica, C.R.; Sitte, H.H.; et al. Powerful Cocaine-like Actions of 3,4-Methylenedioxypyrovalerone (MDPV), a Principal Constituent of Psychoactive “bath Salts” Products. Neuropsychopharmacology 2013, 38, 552–562. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Eshleman, A.J.; Wolfrum, K.M.; Hatfield, M.G.; Johnson, R.A.; Murphy, K.V.; Janowsky, A. Substituted Methcathinones Differ in Transporter and Receptor Interactions. Biochem. Pharmacol. 2013, 85, 1803–1815. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Eshleman, A.J.; Wolfrum, K.M.; Reed, J.F.; Kim, S.O.; Swanson, T.; Johnson, R.A.; Janowsky, A. Structure-Activity Relationships of Substituted Cathinones, with Transporter Binding, Uptake, and Release. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 2017, 360, 33–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Simmler, L.D.; Buser, T.A.; Donzelli, M.; Schramm, Y.; Dieu, L.-H.; Huwyler, J.; Chaboz, S.; Hoener, M.C.; Liechti, M.E. Pharmacological Characterization of Designer Cathinones in Vitro. Br. J. Pharmacol. 2013, 168, 458–470. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Simmler, L.D.; Rickli, A.; Hoener, M.C.; Liechti, M.E. Monoamine Transporter and Receptor Interaction Profiles of a New Series of Designer Cathinones. Neuropharmacology 2014, 79, 152–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Johnson, P.S.; Johnson, M.W. Investigation of “Bath Salts” Use Patterns within an Online Sample of Users in the United States. J. Psychoact. Drugs 2014, 46, 369–378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Kyle, P.B.; Iverson, R.B.; Gajagowni, R.G.; Spencer, L. Illicit Bath Salts: Not for Bathing. J. Miss. State Med. Assoc. 2011, 52, 375–377. [Google Scholar]
  9. Murray, B.L.; Murphy, C.M.; Beuhler, M.C. Death Following Recreational Use of Designer Drug “Bath Salts” Containing 3,4-Methylenedioxypyrovalerone (MDPV). J. Med. Toxicol. 2012, 8, 69–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Spiller, H.A.; Ryan, M.L.; Weston, R.G.; Jansen, J. Clinical Experience with and Analytical Confirmation of “Bath Salts” and “Legal Highs” (Synthetic Cathinones) in the United States. Clin. Toxicol. 2011, 49, 499–505. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Gannon, B.M.; Baumann, M.H.; Walther, D.; Jimenez-Morigosa, C.; Sulima, A.; Rice, K.C.; Collins, G.T. The Abuse-Related Effects of Pyrrolidine-Containing Cathinones Are Related to Their Potency and Selectivity to Inhibit the Dopamine Transporter. Neuropsychopharmacology 2018, 43, 2399–2407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Seely, K.A.; Patton, A.L.; Moran, C.L.; Womack, M.L.; Prather, P.L.; Fantegrossi, W.E.; Radominska-Pandya, A.; Endres, G.W.; Channell, K.B.; Smith, N.H.; et al. Forensic Investigation of K2, Spice, and “Bath Salt” Commercial Preparations: A Three-Year Study of New Designer Drug Products Containing Synthetic Cannabinoid, Stimulant, and Hallucinogenic Compounds. Forensic Sci. Int. 2013, 233, 416–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Shanks, K.G.; Dahn, T.; Behonick, G.; Terrell, A. Analysis of First and Second Generation Legal Highs for Synthetic Cannabinoids and Synthetic Stimulants by Ultra-Performance Liquid Chromatography and Time of Flight Mass Spectrometry. J. Anal. Toxicol. 2012, 36, 360–371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Collins, G.T.; Abbott, M.; Galindo, K.; Rush, E.L.; Rice, K.C.; France, C.P. Discriminative Stimulus Effects of Binary Drug Mixtures: Studies with Cocaine, MDPV, and Caffeine. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 2016, 359, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. DeLarge, A.F.; Erwin, L.L.; Winsauer, P.J. Atypical Binding at Dopamine and Serotonin Transporters Contribute to the Discriminative Stimulus Effects of Mephedrone. Neuropharmacology 2017, 119, 62–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Fantegrossi, W.E.; Gannon, B.M.; Zimmerman, S.M.; Rice, K.C. In Vivo Effects of Abused “bath Salt” Constituent 3,4-Methylenedioxypyrovalerone (MDPV) in Mice: Drug Discrimination, Thermoregulation, and Locomotor Activity. Neuropsychopharmacology 2013, 38, 563–573. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Gannon, B.M.; Williamson, A.; Suzuki, M.; Rice, K.C.; Fantegrossi, W.E. Stereoselective Effects of Abused “Bath Salt” Constituent 3,4-Methylenedioxypyrovalerone in Mice: Drug Discrimination, Locomotor Activity, and Thermoregulation. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 2016, 356, 615–623. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Gatch, M.B.; Taylor, C.M.; Forster, M.J. Locomotor Stimulant and Discriminative Stimulus Effects of “bath Salt” Cathinones. Behav. Pharmacol. 2013, 24, 437–447. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Seaman, R.W.; Doyle, M.R.; Sulima, A.; Rice, K.C.; Collins, G.T. Discriminative Stimulus Effects of 3,4-Methylenedioxypyrovalerone (MDPV) and Structurally Related Synthetic Cathinones. Behav. Pharmacol. 2021, 32, 357–367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Aarde, S.M.; Huang, P.K.; Creehan, K.M.; Dickerson, T.J.; Taffe, M.A. The Novel Recreational Drug 3,4-Methylenedioxypyrovalerone (MDPV) Is a Potent Psychomotor Stimulant: Self-Administration and Locomotor Activity in Rats. Neuropharmacology 2013, 71, 130–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Collins, G.T.; Sulima, A.; Rice, K.C.; France, C.P. Self-Administration of the Synthetic Cathinones 3,4-Methylenedioxypyrovalerone (MDPV) and α-Pyrrolidinopentiophenone (α-PVP) in Rhesus Monkeys. Psychopharmacology 2019, 236, 3677–3685. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. de Moura, F.B.; Sherwood, A.; Prisinzano, T.E.; Paronis, C.A.; Bergman, J.; Kohut, S.J. Reinforcing Effects of Synthetic Cathinones in Rhesus Monkeys: Dose-Response and Behavioral Economic Analyses. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 2021, 202, 173112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Gannon, B.M.; Galindo, K.I.; Rice, K.C.; Collins, G.T. Individual Differences in the Relative Reinforcing Effects of 3,4-Methylenedioxypyrovalerone under Fixed and Progressive Ratio Schedules of Reinforcement in Rats. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 2017, 361, 181–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Seaman, R.W.; Rice, K.C.; Collins, G.T. Relative Reinforcing Effects of Cocaine and 3,4-Methylenedioxypyrovalerone (MDPV) under a Concurrent Access Self-Administration Procedure in Rats. Drug Alcohol. Depend. 2022, 232, 109299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Watterson, L.R.; Kufahl, P.R.; Nemirovsky, N.E.; Sewalia, K.; Grabenauer, M.; Thomas, B.F.; Marusich, J.A.; Wegner, S.; Olive, M.F. Potent Rewarding and Reinforcing Effects of the Synthetic Cathinone 3,4-Methylenedioxypyrovalerone (MDPV). Addict. Biol. 2014, 19, 165–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Coccini, T.; Vecchio, S.; Crevani, M.; De Simone, U. Cytotoxic Effects of 3,4-Catechol-PV (One Major MDPV Metabolite) on Human Dopaminergic SH-SY5Y Cells. Neurotox. Res. 2019, 35, 49–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Leong, H.S.; Philp, M.; Simone, M.; Witting, P.K.; Fu, S. Synthetic Cathinones Induce Cell Death in Dopaminergic SH-SY5Y Cells via Stimulating Mitochondrial Dysfunction. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2020, 21, 1370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Valente, M.J.; Bastos, M.d.L.; Fernandes, E.; Carvalho, F.; Guedes de Pinho, P.; Carvalho, M. Neurotoxicity of β-Keto Amphetamines: Deathly Mechanisms Elicited by Methylone and MDPV in Human Dopaminergic SH-SY5Y Cells. ACS Chem. Neurosci. 2017, 8, 850–859. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Valente, M.J.; Amaral, C.; Correia-da-Silva, G.; Duarte, J.A.; Bastos, M.d.L.; Carvalho, F.; Guedes de Pinho, P.; Carvalho, M. Methylone and MDPV Activate Autophagy in Human Dopaminergic SH-SY5Y Cells: A New Insight into the Context of β-Keto Amphetamines-Related Neurotoxicity. Arch. Toxicol. 2017, 91, 3663–3676. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Wojcieszak, J.; Andrzejczak, D.; Woldan-Tambor, A.; Zawilska, J.B. Cytotoxic Activity of Pyrovalerone Derivatives, an Emerging Group of Psychostimulant Designer Cathinones. Neurotox. Res. 2016, 30, 239–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Rosas-Hernandez, H.; Cuevas, E.; Lantz, S.M.; Rice, K.C.; Gannon, B.M.; Fantegrossi, W.E.; Gonzalez, C.; Paule, M.G.; Ali, S.F. Methamphetamine, 3,4-Methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) and 3,4-Methylenedioxypyrovalerone (MDPV) Induce Differential Cytotoxic Effects in Bovine Brain Microvessel Endothelial Cells. Neurosci. Lett. 2016, 629, 125–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Bernstein, D.L.; Nayak, S.U.; Oliver, C.F.; Rawls, S.M.; Rom, S. Methylenedioxypyrovalerone (MDPV) Impairs Working Memory and Alters Patterns of Dopamine Signaling in Mesocorticolimbic Substrates. Neurosci. Res. 2020, 155, 56–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Brunt, T.M.; Poortman, A.; Niesink, R.J.M.; van den Brink, W. Instability of the Ecstasy Market and a New Kid on the Block: Mephedrone. J. Psychopharmacol. 2011, 25, 1543–1547. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Dargan, P.I.; Albert, S.; Wood, D.M. Mephedrone Use and Associated Adverse Effects in School and College/University Students before the UK Legislation Change. QJM 2010, 103, 875–879. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Lenz, J.; Brown, J.; Flagg, S.; Oh, R.; Batts, K.; Ditzler, T.; Johnson, J. Cristalius: A Case in Designer Drugs. Mil. Med. 2013, 178, e893–e895. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Stoica, M.V.; Felthous, A.R. Acute Psychosis Induced by Bath Salts: A Case Report with Clinical and Forensic Implications. J. Forensic Sci. 2013, 58, 530–533. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Winstock, A.; Mitcheson, L.; Ramsey, J.; Davies, S.; Puchnarewicz, M.; Marsden, J. Mephedrone: Use, Subjective Effects and Health Risks. Addiction 2011, 106, 1991–1996. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Sewalia, K.; Watterson, L.R.; Hryciw, A.; Belloc, A.; Ortiz, J.B.; Olive, M.F. Neurocognitive Dysfunction Following Repeated Binge-like Self-Administration of the Synthetic Cathinone 3,4-Methylenedioxypyrovalerone (MDPV). Neuropharmacology 2018, 134, 36–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Doyle, M.R.; Sulima, A.; Rice, K.C.; Collins, G.T. Interactions between Reinforcement History and Drug-Primed Reinstatement: Studies with MDPV and Mixtures of MDPV and Caffeine. Addict. Biol. 2021, 26, e12904. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Gannon, B.M.; Galindo, K.I.; Mesmin, M.P.; Rice, K.C.; Collins, G.T. Reinforcing Effects of Binary Mixtures of Common Bath Salt Constituents: Studies with 3,4-Methylenedioxypyrovalerone (MDPV), 3,4-Methylenedioxymethcathinone (Methylone), and Caffeine in Rats. Neuropsychopharmacology 2018, 43, 761–769. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Suzuki, M.; Deschamps, J.R.; Jacobson, A.E.; Rice, K.C. Chiral Resolution and Absolute Configuration of the Enantiomers of the Psychoactive “Designer Drug” 3,4-Methylenedioxypyrovalerone. Chirality 2015, 27, 287–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Ennaceur, A.; Delacour, J. A New One-Trial Test for Neurobiological Studies of Memory in Rats. 1: Behavioral Data. Behav. Brain Res. 1988, 31, 47–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Murnane, K.S.; Perrine, S.A.; Finton, B.J.; Galloway, M.P.; Howell, L.L.; Fantegrossi, W.E. Effects of Exposure to Amphetamine Derivatives on Passive Avoidance Performance and the Central Levels of Monoamines and Their Metabolites in Mice: Correlations between Behavior and Neurochemistry. Psychopharmacology 2012, 220, 495–508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Scott, C.C.; Chen, K.K. Comparison of the Action of 1-Ethyl Theobromine and Caffeine in Animals and Man. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 1944, 82, 89–97. [Google Scholar]
  45. Bradley, C.A.; Palmatier, M.I. Intravenous and Oral Caffeine Self-Administration in Rats. Drug Alcohol. Depend. 2019, 203, 72–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Briscoe, R.J.; Vanecek, S.A.; Vallett, M.; Baird, T.J.; Holloway, F.A.; Gauvin, D.V. Reinforcing Effects of Caffeine, Ephedrine, and Their Binary Combination in Rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 1998, 60, 685–693. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Gannon, B.M.; Mesmin, M.P.; Sulima, A.; Rice, K.C.; Collins, G.T. Behavioral Economic Analysis of the Reinforcing Effects of “Bath Salts” Mixtures: Studies with MDPV, Methylone, and Caffeine in Male Sprague-Dawley Rats. Psychopharmacology 2019, 236, 1031–1041. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Prieto, J.P.; Scorza, C.; Serra, G.P.; Perra, V.; Galvalisi, M.; Abin-Carriquiry, J.A.; Piras, G.; Valentini, V. Caffeine, a Common Active Adulterant of Cocaine, Enhances the Reinforcing Effect of Cocaine and Its Motivational Value. Psychopharmacology 2016, 233, 2879–2889. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Peters, J.; Scofield, M.D.; Reichel, C.M. Chemogenetic Activation of the Perirhinal Cortex Reverses Methamphetamine-Induced Memory Deficits and Reduces Relapse. Learn. Mem. 2018, 25, 410–415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Reichel, C.M.; Schwendt, M.; McGinty, J.F.; Olive, M.F.; See, R.E. Loss of Object Recognition Memory Produced by Extended Access to Methamphetamine Self-Administration Is Reversed by Positive Allosteric Modulation of Metabotropic Glutamate Receptor 5. Neuropsychopharmacology 2011, 36, 782–792. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Reichel, C.M.; Ramsey, L.A.; Schwendt, M.; McGinty, J.F.; See, R.E. Methamphetamine-Induced Changes in the Object Recognition Memory Circuit. Neuropharmacology 2012, 62, 1119–1126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Rogers, J.L.; De Santis, S.; See, R.E. Extended Methamphetamine Self-Administration Enhances Reinstatement of Drug Seeking and Impairs Novel Object Recognition in Rats. Psychopharmacology 2008, 199, 615–624. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Scofield, M.D.; Trantham-Davidson, H.; Schwendt, M.; Leong, K.-C.; Peters, J.; See, R.E.; Reichel, C.M. Failure to Recognize Novelty after Extended Methamphetamine Self-Administration Results from Loss of Long-Term Depression in the Perirhinal Cortex. Neuropsychopharmacology 2015, 40, 2526–2535. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Schweppe, C.A.; Burzynski, C.; Jayanthi, S.; Ladenheim, B.; Cadet, J.L.; Gardner, E.L.; Xi, Z.-X.; van Praag, H.; Newman, A.H.; Keck, T.M. Neurochemical and Behavioral Comparisons of Contingent and Non-Contingent Methamphetamine Exposure Following Binge or Yoked Long-Access Self-Administration Paradigms. Psychopharmacology 2020, 237, 1989–2005. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Briand, L.A.; Gross, J.P.; Robinson, T.E. Impaired Object Recognition Following Prolonged Withdrawal from Extended-Access Cocaine Self-Administration. Neuroscience 2008, 155, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Gobin, C.; Schwendt, M. The Effects of Extended-Access Cocaine Self-Administration on Working Memory Performance, Reversal Learning and Incubation of Cocaine-Seeking in Adult Male Rats. J. Addict. Prev. 2017, 5, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Abreu, R.V.; Silva-Oliveira, E.M.; Moraes, M.F.D.; Pereira, G.S.; Moraes-Santos, T. Chronic Coffee and Caffeine Ingestion Effects on the Cognitive Function and Antioxidant System of Rat Brains. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 2011, 99, 659–664. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Angelucci, M.E.M.; Cesário, C.; Hiroi, R.H.; Rosalen, P.L.; Da Cunha, C. Effects of Caffeine on Learning and Memory in Rats Tested in the Morris Water Maze. Braz. J. Med. Biol. Res. 2002, 35, 1201–1208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Assis, M.S.; Soares, A.C.; Sousa, D.N.; Eudes-Filho, J.; Faro, L.R.F.; Carneiro, F.P.; Silva, M.V.; Motoyama, A.B.; Souza, G.M.R.; Marchiori, S.; et al. Effects of Caffeine on Behavioural and Cognitive Deficits in Rats. Basic. Clin. Pharmacol. Toxicol. 2018, 123, 435–442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Cognato, G.P.; Agostinho, P.M.; Hockemeyer, J.; Müller, C.E.; Souza, D.O.; Cunha, R.A. Caffeine and an Adenosine A(2A) Receptor Antagonist Prevent Memory Impairment and Synaptotoxicity in Adult Rats Triggered by a Convulsive Episode in Early Life. J. Neurochem. 2010, 112, 453–462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Espinosa, J.; Rocha, A.; Nunes, F.; Costa, M.S.; Schein, V.; Kazlauckas, V.; Kalinine, E.; Souza, D.O.; Cunha, R.A.; Porciúncula, L.O. Caffeine Consumption Prevents Memory Impairment, Neuronal Damage, and Adenosine A2A Receptors Upregulation in the Hippocampus of a Rat Model of Sporadic Dementia. J. Alzheimers Dis. 2013, 34, 509–518. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Imam-Fulani, A.; Owoyele, B.V. Effect of Caffeine and Adrenaline on Memory and Anxiety in Male Wistar Rats. Niger. J. Physiol. Sci. 2022, 37, 69–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Kasimay Cakir, O.; Ellek, N.; Salehin, N.; Hamamcı, R.; Keleş, H.; Kayalı, D.G.; Akakın, D.; Yüksel, M.; Özbeyli, D. Protective Effect of Low Dose Caffeine on Psychological Stress and Cognitive Function. Physiol. Behav. 2017, 168, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Leite, M.R.; Wilhelm, E.A.; Jesse, C.R.; Brandão, R.; Nogueira, C.W. Protective Effect of Caffeine and a Selective A2A Receptor Antagonist on Impairment of Memory and Oxidative Stress of Aged Rats. Exp. Gerontol. 2011, 46, 309–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Leite, M.R.; Marcondes Sari, M.H.; de Freitas, M.L.; Oliveira, L.P.; Dalmolin, L.; Brandão, R.; Zeni, G. Caffeine and Diphenyl Diselenide Improve Long-Term Memory Impaired in Middle-Aged Rats. Exp. Gerontol. 2014, 53, 67–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Ozawa, T.; Kaseda, K.; Ichitani, Y.; Yamada, K. Caffeine Facilitates Extinction of Auditory Fear Conditioning in Rats. Neuropsychopharmacol. Rep. 2022, 42, 521–525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Petkov, V.D.; Konstantinova, E.; Petkov, V.V.; Lazarova, M.; Petkova, B. Modulation of the Effects on Learning and Memory of Nootropic Drugs and Central Stimulants When Applied Together. Acta Physiol. Pharmacol. Bulg. 1991, 17, 17–26. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  68. Roussinov, K.S.; Yonkov, D.I. Cholinergic Mechanisms in the Learning and Memory Facilitating Effect of Caffeine. Acta Physiol. Pharmacol. Bulg. 1976, 2, 61–68. [Google Scholar]
  69. Takahashi, R.N.; Pamplona, F.A.; Prediger, R.D.S. Adenosine Receptor Antagonists for Cognitive Dysfunction: A Review of Animal Studies. Front. Biosci. 2008, 13, 2614–2632. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Cadet, J.L.; Krasnova, I.N.; Jayanthi, S.; Lyles, J. Neurotoxicity of Substituted Amphetamines: Molecular and Cellular Mechanisms. Neurotox. Res. 2007, 11, 183–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Brennan, K.A.; Colussi-Mas, J.; Carati, C.; Lea, R.A.; Fitzmaurice, P.S.; Schenk, S. Methamphetamine Self-Administration and the Effect of Contingency on Monoamine and Metabolite Tissue Levels in the Rat. Brain Res. 2010, 1317, 137–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  72. Krasnova, I.N.; Justinova, Z.; Ladenheim, B.; Jayanthi, S.; McCoy, M.T.; Barnes, C.; Warner, J.E.; Goldberg, S.R.; Cadet, J.L. Methamphetamine Self-Administration Is Associated with Persistent Biochemical Alterations in Striatal and Cortical Dopaminergic Terminals in the Rat. PLoS ONE 2010, 5, e8790. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Schwendt, M.; Rocha, A.; See, R.E.; Pacchioni, A.M.; McGinty, J.F.; Kalivas, P.W. Extended Methamphetamine Self-Administration in Rats Results in a Selective Reduction of Dopamine Transporter Levels in the Prefrontal Cortex and Dorsal Striatum Not Accompanied by Marked Monoaminergic Depletion. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 2009, 331, 555–562. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Shepard, J.D.; Chuang, D.T.; Shaham, Y.; Morales, M. Effect of Methamphetamine Self-Administration on Tyrosine Hydroxylase and Dopamine Transporter Levels in Mesolimbic and Nigrostriatal Dopamine Pathways of the Rat. Psychopharmacology 2006, 185, 505–513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Average drug intake (or number of pellet deliveries for rats that had access to saline and grain pellets) (left ordinate) per session as a function of experimental week. Total experimental intake in rats that had access to each reinforcer for 90 min or 12 h is represented on the right ordinate. Triangles represent rats that had 12 h access, and circles represent rats that had access for 90 min. Data represent the mean ± S.E.M. ** denotes p < 0.01, *** denotes p < 0.001.
Figure 1. Average drug intake (or number of pellet deliveries for rats that had access to saline and grain pellets) (left ordinate) per session as a function of experimental week. Total experimental intake in rats that had access to each reinforcer for 90 min or 12 h is represented on the right ordinate. Triangles represent rats that had 12 h access, and circles represent rats that had access for 90 min. Data represent the mean ± S.E.M. ** denotes p < 0.01, *** denotes p < 0.001.
Brainsci 14 00258 g001
Figure 2. Daily drug intake (or number of pellet deliveries for rats that had access to saline and grain pellets) over the course of the experiment. Triangles represent rats that had 12 h access, and circles represent rats that had 90 min access. Data represent the mean ± S.E.M.
Figure 2. Daily drug intake (or number of pellet deliveries for rats that had access to saline and grain pellets) over the course of the experiment. Triangles represent rats that had 12 h access, and circles represent rats that had 90 min access. Data represent the mean ± S.E.M.
Brainsci 14 00258 g002
Figure 3. Average NOR score and total exploration time at baseline (prior to any drug exposure) (open squares) versus following a three-week drug-free period (closed squares). Data represent the mean ± S.E.M. * denotes p < 0.05.
Figure 3. Average NOR score and total exploration time at baseline (prior to any drug exposure) (open squares) versus following a three-week drug-free period (closed squares). Data represent the mean ± S.E.M. * denotes p < 0.05.
Brainsci 14 00258 g003
Figure 4. Average levels of dopamine and dopamine metabolites as a function of both reinforcer and access duration. Data represent the mean ± S.E.M. * denotes p < 0.05, ** denotes p < 0.01, *** denotes p < 0.001, **** denotes p < 0.0001.
Figure 4. Average levels of dopamine and dopamine metabolites as a function of both reinforcer and access duration. Data represent the mean ± S.E.M. * denotes p < 0.05, ** denotes p < 0.01, *** denotes p < 0.001, **** denotes p < 0.0001.
Brainsci 14 00258 g004
Table 1. Average levels of norepinephrine, serotonin, and their metabolites as a function of both reinforcer and access duration. Data represent the mean ± S.E.M.
Table 1. Average levels of norepinephrine, serotonin, and their metabolites as a function of both reinforcer and access duration. Data represent the mean ± S.E.M.
Norepinephrine
(ng/mg)
Epinephrine
(ng/mg)
Norepinephrine Turnover (Epinephrine/Norepinephrine)Serotonin
(ng/mg)
5-HIAA
(ng/mg)
Serotonin Turnover
(5-HIAA/Serotonin)
90-min12-h90-min12-h90-min12-h90-min12-h90-min12-h90-min12-h
Food0.15 ± 0.020.35 ± 0.070.23 ± 0.040.15 ± 0.051.8 ± 0.410.61 ± 0.182.6 ± 0.53.2 ± 0.41.5 ± 0.221.3 ± 0.141.1 ± 0.350.39 ± 0.09
Cocaine0.32 ± 0.070.32 ± 0.040.09 ± 0.020.07 ± 0.030.4 ± 0.130.21 ± 0.091.2 ± 0.161.22 ± 0.061.5 ± 0.181.4 ± 0.121.3 ± 0.131 ± 0.15
MDPV0.26 ± 0.040.28 ± 0.040.09 ± 0.040.1 0 ± 0.030.23 ± 0.10.28 ± 0.11.2 ± 0.111 ± 0.041.5 ± 0.141.1 ± 0.191.3 ± 0.210.71 ± 0.05
Methamphetamine0.80 ± 0.140.85 ± 0.090.01 ± 0.0030.02 ± 0.010.02 ± 0.0050.03 ± 0.011.4 ± 0.11.5 ± 0.091.3 ± 0.21.39 ± 0.120.98 ± 0.140.93 ± 0.07
MDPV+Caffeine0.32 ± 0.050.45 ± 0.080.07 ± 0.020 ± 00.32 ± 0.050.45 ± 0.081.2 ± 0.171.2 ± 0.11.6 ± 0.091.7 ± 0.161.5 ± 0.131.3 ± 0.2
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Seaman, R.W., Jr.; Lamon, K.; Whitton, N.; Latimer, B.; Sulima, A.; Rice, K.C.; Murnane, K.S.; Collins, G.T. Impacts of Self-Administered 3,4-Methylenedioxypyrovalerone (MDPV) Alone, and in Combination with Caffeine, on Recognition Memory and Striatal Monoamine Neurochemistry in Male Sprague Dawley Rats: Comparisons with Methamphetamine and Cocaine. Brain Sci. 2024, 14, 258. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14030258

AMA Style

Seaman RW Jr., Lamon K, Whitton N, Latimer B, Sulima A, Rice KC, Murnane KS, Collins GT. Impacts of Self-Administered 3,4-Methylenedioxypyrovalerone (MDPV) Alone, and in Combination with Caffeine, on Recognition Memory and Striatal Monoamine Neurochemistry in Male Sprague Dawley Rats: Comparisons with Methamphetamine and Cocaine. Brain Sciences. 2024; 14(3):258. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14030258

Chicago/Turabian Style

Seaman, Robert W., Jr., Kariann Lamon, Nicholas Whitton, Brian Latimer, Agnieszka Sulima, Kenner C. Rice, Kevin S. Murnane, and Gregory T. Collins. 2024. "Impacts of Self-Administered 3,4-Methylenedioxypyrovalerone (MDPV) Alone, and in Combination with Caffeine, on Recognition Memory and Striatal Monoamine Neurochemistry in Male Sprague Dawley Rats: Comparisons with Methamphetamine and Cocaine" Brain Sciences 14, no. 3: 258. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14030258

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop